***************
Invited Symposium: Neural Bases of Hypnosis






Abstract

Section 1

Section 2

Section 3

Section 4

Section 5




Discussion
Board

INABIS '98 Home Page Your Session Symposia & Poster Sessions Plenary Sessions Exhibitors' Foyer Personal Itinerary New Search

Brain Dynamic Shifts During The Elimination Of Perceived Pain And Distress: Neuroimaging Studies of Hypnotic Analgesia


Contact Person: Helen J. Crawford (hjc@vt.edu)


Introduction

Pain is a dynamic, multifaceted experience comprised of sensory-discriminative, motivational-affective, cognitive and motoric components (e.g., Melzack, 1992; Melzack & Wall, 1965; Price, 1988). Psychological components such as attentional deployment, stress, and past experiences can have a major impact on the degree to which pain is experienced. It is well established that hypnotically suggested analgesia is highly effective in the reduction or elimination of acute and chronic pain, especially in moderately to highly hypnotizable individuals (Crawford, 1994a,b; in press; Hilgard, & Hilgard, 1994). Yet, only recently have we begun to unravel the underlying neurophysiological processes that are involved in hypnotic analgesia.

The aim of this paper is to review recent research, particularly from our laboratory, that supports our proposal that hypnotic analgesia is an active inhibitory process that depends upon a supervisory attentional control system that operates to reallocate thalamocortical activities so that incoming painful stimuli are suppressed at cortical and subcortical levels and do not enter conscious awareness. Furthermore, we provide evidence that highly hypnotizable persons can better control pain because of their more effective frontal attention system that permits them to either attend to or disattend incoming stimuli.

Back to the top.


General Review

Hypnotic analgesia is attention-based in that persons inhibit incoming sensations from awareness while often simultaneously deploying their attention elsewhere. Somewhat paradoxically, some highs can also attend to and describe their hand being in normally painful ice cold water while continuing to have no awareness of distress or pain (unpublished data); this suggests a strong dissociative aspect of hypnotic analgesia (Hilgard, 1986; Hilgard & Hilgard, 1994). Most important in any consideration of hypnotic analgesia is the moderating effect of hypnotic susceptibility.

Hypnotic susceptibility is an enduring trait (Piccione, Hilgard, & Zimbard, 1989) that is, like the tip of an iceberg, reflective of underlying individual differences in information processing. In contrast to hypnotically nonresponsive persons ("lows"), highly hypnotizable persons ("highs") have a greater disposition for more focused and sustained attention, deeper absorptive involvement in experiences be they positive or negative in nature, and greater cognitive flexibility that is the ability to shift from one strategy to another or from one alternate state of consciousness to another (Crawford, 1989; Crawford & Allen, 1983; Crawford, Brown, & Moon, 1993; Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992; Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974). Highs show superior performance on certain attention tasks (for review, see Crawford 1994b) and faster reaction times to complex decision-making tasks (Crawford, Horton, & Lamas, 1998).

Furthermore, highs may have significantly shorter latencies for certain somatosensory and auditory event-related potentials (Crawford et al., under review; Crawford, Horton, & Lamas, 1998; Lamas, Crawford, & Vendemia, 1997; Lamas & Crawford, 1998). A rather robust finding is that highs generate significantly more theta EEG activity than lows. This theta activity may originate in the hippocampal region and be associated with focused attentional and inhibitory processes (e.g., Crawford, 1990; Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992; Sabourin et al., 1990). Furthermore, Crawford, Clarke, and Kitner-Triolo (1996) found highs had greater enhanced power in upper theta, upper alpha, and mid beta (around 20 Hz) bands -- all bands that are sometimes associated with attentional processing in the literature. In conjunction with behavioral data, these neurophysiological data provide support for the proposal that highs have more effective attention processing systems than lows in normal every-day experiences as well as during hyp nosis. Hypnosis often involves an amplification of focused attention and inhibition that draws upon these underlying abilities seen in highs.

How then does hypnotic analgesia affect the neurophysiological representation of pain? In its most elementary representation, pain is a nociceptive spinal reflex (Sherrington, 1947). Once within the brain complex neural networks are involved including: 1) a sensory-discriminative network that involves the primary (S1) and secondary (S2) somatosensory cortices, thalamus, and insula; 2) an attentional network that includes the anterior cingulate and anterior regions of the brain interacting with other brain regions; 3) a motor network that includes the premotor cortex, frontal oculomotor fields, putamen, globus pallidus, red nucleus and superior colliculus; and 4) a descending control network involving the periaqueductal grey and higher cortical areas (e.g., Iadarola et al., 1998). Of great important to hypnotic analgesia are the descending inhibitory systems that modulate sensory input such as higher cortical centers, the midbrain, the periaqueductal grey area, and the spinal cord (for reviews see Handwer ker & Kobal, 1993; Willis & Westlund, 1997).

Spinal Level. Hypnotic analgesia apparently has an inhibitory effect on peripheral spinal reflex activity (for review, see Price, 1996). Changes in both the latency and amplitude of spinal reflexes have been observed (Danzinger et al., 1998; Hagbarth & Finer, 1963; Santacangelo, Busse, & Carli, 1989; Kiernan, Dane, Phillips, & Price, 1995).

Neurochemical processes. Since some inhibitory processes operate via endogenous opioid peptides, the effect of hypnotic analgesia on the opiate descending control mechanism has been investigated. Hypnotic analgesia has generally not been reversed by the opiate antagonist naloxone (Barber & Mayer, 1977; Goldstein & Hilgard, 1975; Spiegel & Albert, 1983; but see Stevenson, 1978) except under circumstances of stress (Frid & Singer, 1978). Preliminary studies (e.g., Domangue, Margolis, Lieberman, & Kaji, 1985; Sternbach, 1982) suggest other neurochemical processes may be involved in hypnotic analgesia. The mean plasma level of beta-endorphin-immunoreactivity was enhanced after hypnotic analgesia in arthritic patients, with no significant changes in plasma levels of epinephrine, dopamine or serotonin (Domangue et al., 1985).

EEG Activity. The background EEG during ongoing tonic pain is impacted by hypnotic analgesia. In a patient undergoing dental surgery with only hypnotic analgesia, Chen, Dworkin, and Bloomquist (1981) found that the left hemisphere alpha and theta EEG power decreased. Karlin, Morgan, and Goldstein (1980) reported greater right parieto-occipital involvement, as assessed by total EEG power, to cold pressor pain (immersion of hand into ice cold water 1 - 2 degrees C).

In another cold pressor pain study, Crawford (1990) reported an interaction between conditions (attend and hypnotic analgesia during hypnosis) and hypnotic level in the upper theta (5.5 - 7.5 Hz) frequency band. Overall, highs generated substantially more theta power than did lows at midfrontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital sites. In the temporal region (T3, T4), when attending to cold pressor pain to the left hand, highs had significantly more left hemisphere dominant high theta power. By contrast, during hypnotic analgesia highs showed a significant reduction in left hemisphere power and an increased in right hemisphere power in the upper theta band. Lows showed no hemispheric differences and remained similar in both conditions. De Pascalis and Perrone (1996) reported significant reductions of delta, and beta in the right hemisphere in stimulus-bound EEG following electric shocks to the left wrist in highs but not lows.

Event-Related Potentials. The amplitude of somatosensory ERPs can be affected by attentional manipulations. Thus it is not surprising there are significant decreases in the amplitudes of late SERP components in response to unpleasant cutaneous stimulation occurring during hypnotic analgesia (e.g., Arendt-Nielsen, Zachariae, & Bjerring, 1990; Crawford, 1994a; Crawford et al., 1998b; De Pascalis & Carboni, 1997; De Pascalis, Crawford, & Marucci, 1992; Mészáros, Bányai, & Greguss, 1980; Miltner, Johnson, Braun, & Larbig, 1989; Sharev & Tal, 1989; Spiegel, Bierre, & Rootenberg, 1989; Zachariae & Bjerring, 1990, 1994; Zachariae, Bjerring, Arendt-Nielsen, Nielsen, & Gotliebsen, 1991). However, there are exceptions (e.g., Meier, Klucken, Soyka, & Bromm, 1993). Inconsistent findings may be due in part to the choice of SERP components (P100, P250, P200, P300, N140, and/or N150-P250 complex), recordings often being limited to vertex and nearby regions, and varying intensities of painful stimulation among st udies.

Recently our laboratory has also been documenting the effect of hypnotic analgesia upon the anterior frontal (prefrontal) region since recent neuroimaging studies document this region to be important in pain processing (e.g., Crawford, Gur, Skolnick, Gur, & Benson, 1993; Davis, Taylor, Crawley, Wood, & Mikulis, 1997; Davis, Wood, Crawley, & Millulis, 1995; Derbyshire et al., 1996; Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, 1995; Iadarola et al., 1998; Jones, Brown, Friston, Qi & Frackowiak, 1991; Talbot, Marrett, Evans, Meyer, Bushnell, & Duncan, 1991). The anterior frontal cortex has been shown to control input to the more posterior systems of the cortex (e.g., Skinner & Yingling, 1977) and gate the early stages of somatosensory processing (e.g., Desmedt & Tomberg, 1989; Yamaguchi & Knight, 1990).

In a study of moderately to highly hypnotizable persons with chronic low back pain, Crawford et al. (1998b) documented not only reduced P200 and P300 amplitudes more posteriorly during hypnotic analgesia, but also anterior frontal changes. The anterior frontal changes included an enhanced N140 and a pre-stimulus positive-going contingent variation at the left anterior frontal (Fp1) region only. Unlike the classic contingent negative variation, a positive-moving contingent variation may result from inhibitory processing and "a disfacilitation in cortical neuronal networks" (Rockstroh et al., 1993, p. 236; for reviews, see Birbaumer et al., 1990; Tecce & Cattanach, 1982). More recently, in a study of healthy young adults (Crawford et al., under review) reductions of P70 amplitudes in the far anterior frontal region along with reduced P200 and P300 amplitudes more posteriorly in highs but not lows. Enhancements of N140 and N250, associated with inhibitory processing, were observed. Horton, McClain-Furmanski , Mészáros, and Crawford (1998) replicated these findings.

Kropotov, Crawford, and Polyakov (1997) recorded SERPs from temporarily implanted intracranial electrodes to noxious electrical stimuli presented to the right middle finger during attention and hypnotically induced analgesia. SERPs were recorded in the left anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, temporal and parietal cortices of two patients. In the hypnotically responsive patient a significant reduction of the positive SERP component within the range of 140-160 ms poststimulus was recorded in the left anterior cingulate cortex during reduced pain perception. Additionally, there was a significant enhancement of the negative SERP component within the range of 200-260 ms recorded in the left anterior temporal cortex. These data further support our proposal that active inhibitory processing occurs during hypnotic analgesia.

Neuroimaging Studies. With the advent of various neuroimaging techniques including regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) metabolism, positron emission tomography (PET), and functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), a new window on brain processes has been opened. The identification of multiple neural networks associated with pain processing is actively being explored by a number of laboratories, yet only a few studies have examined the effect of hypnotic analgesia using these new techniques.

Changes in rCBF metabolism, as measured by the 133-xenon method, accompanying rest, ischemic pain without suggested analgesia, and ischemic pain with suggested analgesia conditions in sessions with and without hypnosis were investigated by Crawford, Gur, Skolnick, Gur and Benson (1993). Training in applying techniques of hypnotic analgesia to both cold pressor pain and ischemic pain preceded the rCBF study. As is the practice in our research protocol, to be included in the research highs had to consistently eliminate all pain perception in these training sessions. Following a hypnotic induction, highs (but not lows) showed a dramatic increase in rCBF that may reflect increased cognitive effort or arousal during hypnosis. During hypnotic analgesia highs showed increased bilateral rCBF activation in the anterior frontal cortex as well as the somatosensory cortex. Although one cannot discriminate between excitatory and inhibitory processes with this technique, Crawford et al. (1993) interpreted the increas ed anterior frontal rCBF to support our proposal that greater inhibitory processing occurs in these regions during hypnotic analgesia.

In the first fMRI study of hypnotic analgesia, we (Crawford, Horton, Harrington, Vendemia, Plantec, Yung, Shamro, & Downs, 1998; Crawford, Horton, Hirsch, Harrington, McClain-Furmanski, Shamro, & Downs, 1998) reported dynamic shifts in anterior and posterior regional activation between attention to and hypnotic analgesia of electrical stimulation, at touch vs strong pain levels, that were administered to the left middle finger among highly hypnotizable persons. Most importantly, during hypnotic analgesia we observed dynamic shifts (including reduced right hemisphere activation) in anterior cingulate activation. In a PET study using hypnotic suggestions to reduce distress but not sensory components of pain, Rainville et al. (1997) reported reductions in anterior cigulate activation correlated significantly with reduced perceptions of distress. Prior research has shown that the anterior cingulate has reciprocal connections with the thalamus and anterior frontal region, as well as other brain regions. It is involved in the attention pain system that is more closely associated with the affective pain component (Davis et al., 1995, 1997; Iadarola et al., 1998; Rainville et al., 1997), attentional deployment (Posner & Petersen, 1990), and the execution and suppression of motoric responses (for a review, see Devinksy, Morrell, & Vogt, 1995). Reductions in fMRI activity were observed in insula and thalamic regions, as well as frontal regions of the brain.

Back to the top.


Conclusions

The growing body of literature supports our evolving neurophysiological model of attentional processes (Crawford, 1994a, b; Crawford et al., 1997; 1998a,b; Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992; Kropotov et al., 1997) that proposes that hypnotic analgesia is an active inhibitory process that operates to reallocate thalamocortical activities. Furthermore, the research supports the more general proposal that highly hypnotizable persons have more effective attentional and inhibitory systems than low hypnotizable persons. The application of hypnotic analgesia to neuropathic pain has recently shown accompanying neurophysiological changes in chronic low back pain (Crawford et al., 1998b) and fibromyalgia (Wik, Fischer, Gragee, Finer, & Fredrikson, 1998) populations, with exciting implications for its use in the early treatment of chronic pain and its possible intervention so that chronic pain does not develop.

Back to the top.


References

Arendt-Nielsen, N. L., Zacharie, R., & Bjerring, P. (1990). Quantitative evaluation of hypnotically suggested hyperaesthesia and analgesia by painful laser stimulation. Pain, 42, 243-251.

Barber, J., & Meyer, D. (1977). Evaluation of the efficacy and neural mechanism of a hypnotic analgesia procedure in experimental and clinical dental pain. Pain, 4, 4-48.

Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Canavan, A.G.M., & Rockstroh, B. (1990). Slow potentials of the cerebral cortex and behavior. Physiological Review, 70, 1-41.

Chen, A. C. N., Dworkin, S. F., & Bloomquist, D. S. (1981). Cortical power spectrum analysis of hypnotic pain control in surgery. International Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 127-136.

Crawford, H. J. (1989). Cognitive and physiological flexibility: Multiple pathways to hypnotic responsiveness. In V. Ghorghui, P. Netter, H. Eysenck, & R. Rosenthal (Eds.), Suggestion and suggestibility: Theory and research (pp. 155-168). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Crawford, H. J. (1990). Cognitive and psychophysiological correlates of hypnotic responsiveness and hypnosis. In M. L. Fass & D. Brown (Eds.), Creative mastery in hypnosis and hypnoanalysis: A festschrift for Erika Fromm (pp. 155-168). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

Crawford, H. J. (1994a). Brain systems involved in attention and disattention (hypnotic analgesia) to pain. In K. Pribram (Ed.), Origins: Brain and self organization (pp. 661-679). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Crawford, H. J. (1994b). Brain dynamics and hypnosis: Attentional and disattentional processes. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 4204-232.

Crawford, H. J. (in press). Neuropsychophysiology of hypnosis: Towards an understanding of how hypnotic interventions work. In G. D. Burrows, R. O. Stanley, & P. B. Bloom (Eds.), Advances in Clinical Hypnosis. New York: Wiley.

Crawford, H. J., & Allen, S. N. (1983). Enhanced visual memory during hypnosis as mediated by hypnotic responsiveness and cognitive strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 662-685.

Crawford, H. J., Brown, A., & Moon, C. (1993). Sustained attentional and disattentional abilities: Differences between low and highly hypnotizable persons. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 534-543.

Crawford, H. J., Clarke, S. W., & Kitner-Triolo, M. (1996). Self-generated happy and sad emotions in low and highly hypnotizable persons during waking and hypnosis: laterality and regional EEG activity differences. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, 239-266.

Crawford, H. J., & Gruzelier, J. H. (1992). A midstream view of the neuropsychophysiology of hypnosis: Recent research and future directions. In E. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 227-266). NY: Guilford Press.

Crawford, H. J., Gur, R. C., Skolnick, B., Gur, R. E., & Benson, D. (1993). Effects of hypnosis on regional cerebral blood flow during ischemic pain with and without suggested hypnotic analgesia. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 181-195.

Crawford, H. J., Horton, J. E., Harrington, G. S., Vendemia, J. M. C., Plantec, M. B., Yung, S., Shamro, C., & Downs III, J. H. (1998a). Hypnotic analgesia (disattending pain) impacts neuronal network activation: An fMRI study of noxious somatosensory TENS stimuli. NeuroImage, 7, S436.

Crawford, H. J., Horton, J. E., Hirsch, T. B., Harrington, G. S., Plantec, M. B., Vendemia, J. M. C., Shamro, C., McClain-Furmanski, D., & Downs III, J. H. (1998). Attention and disattention (hypnotic analgesia) to painful somatosensory TENS stimuli differentially affects brain dynamics: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 77.

Crawford, H. J., Horton, J. E., & Lamas, J. (1998). Information processing speed is faster for highly hypnotizable than low hypnotizable persons: Evidence from behavioral reaction time and event-related potential studies. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 84.

Crawford, H. J., Knebel, T., & Vendemia, J. M. C. (1998b). The nature of hypnotic analgesia: Neurophysiological foundation and evidence. Contemporary Hypnosis, 15, 24-35.

Crawford, H. J., Knebel, T., Kaplan, L., Vendemia, J. M. C., Xie, M., Jamison, S., & Pribram, K. H. (1998d). Hypnotic Analgesia: I. Somatosensory Event-Related Potential Changes to Noxious Stimuli and II. Transfer Learning to Reduce Chronic Low Back Pain. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, 92-132.

Danziger, N., et al. (1998). Different strategies of modulation can be operative during hypnotic analgesia: a neurophysiological study. Pain, 75, 85-92.

Davis, K. D., Taylor, S. J., Crawley, A. P., Wood, M. L., & Mikulis, D. J. (1997). Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 3370-3380.

Davis, K.D., Wood, M. L., Crawley, A. P., & Mikulis, D. J. (1995). fMRI of human somatosensory and cingulate cortex during painful electrical nerve stimulation. Neuroreport, 7, 321-325.

De Pascalis, V., & Carboni, G. (1997). P300 event-related-potential amplitudes and evoked cardiac responses during hypnotic alteration of somatosensory perception. International Journal of Neuroscience, 92, 187-207.

De Pascalis, V., Crawford, H. J., & Marucci, F. S. (1992). Analgesia ipnotica nella modulazione del dolore: Effeti sui potenziali somatosensoriali. [The modulation of pain by hypnotic analgesia: Effect on somatosensory evoked potentials.] Comunicazioni Scientifiche di Psicologie Generale, 71-89.

De Pascalis, V., & Perrone, M. (1996). EEG asymmetry and heart rate during experience of hypnotic analgesia in high and low hypnotizables. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 163-175.

Derbyshire, S. W. G., Chen, A. C. N., & Jones, A. K. P. (1996). Somatosensory and frontal cortical processing during pain experience. In O. Franzén, R. Johansson, & L. Terenius (Eds.), Somesthesis and the neurobiology of the somatosensory cortex (pp. 385-394). Basel, Switzerland: Birkhäuser Verlag.

Desmedt, J. E., & Tomberg, C. (1989). Mapping early somatosensory evoked potentials in selective attention: critical evaluation of control conditions using for titrating by difference the cognitive P30, P40, P100 and N140. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 74, 321-346.

Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118, 279-306.

Domangue, B. B., Margolis, C. G., Lieberman, D., & Kaji, H. (1985). Biochemical correlates of hypnoanalgesia in arthritic pain patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 46, 235-238.

Frid, M., & Singer, G. (1980). The effects of naloxone on human pain reactions during stress. In C. Peck & M. Wallace (Eds.), Problems in pain: Proceedings of the first Australian New Zealand conference on pain (pp. 78-86). Sydney: Pergamon Press.

Goldstein, A., & Hilgard, E. R. (1975). Failure of opiate antagonist naloxone to modify hypnotic analgesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 72, 2041-2043.

Hagbarth, K. E., & Finer, B. L. (1963). The plasticity of human withdrawal reflexes to noxious skin stimuli in lower limbs. Progress in Brain Research, 1, 65-78.

Handwerker, H. O., & Kobal, G. (1993). Psychophysiology of experimentally induced pain. Physiological Review, 73, 639-671. Hilgard, E. R. (1986). Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action. New York: Wiley.

Hilgard, E. R., & Hilgard, J. R. (1994). Hypnosis in the relief of pain (rev. ed.). New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc.

Horton, J. E., McClain-Furmanski, D., Mészáros, I., & Crawford, H. J. (1998) To inhibit pain is to actively shift conscious awareness: Somatosensory event-related potential evidence during hypnotic analgesia. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 234.

Iadarola, M. J., Berman, K. F., Zeffiro, T. A., Byas-Smith, M. B., Gracely, R. H., Max, M. B., & Bennett, G. J. (1998). Neural activation during acute capsaicin-evoked pain and allodynia assessed with PET. Brain, 121, 931-947.

Jones, A. K. P., Brown, W. D., Friston, K. J., Qi, L. Y., & Frackowiak, S. J. (1991). Cortical and subcortical localization of response in pain in man using positron emission tomography. Proceedings of the Royal Society London, Series B., Biological Sciences, 244, 39-44.

Karlin, R., Morgan, D., & Goldstein, L. (1980). Hypnotic analgesia: A preliminary investigation of quantitated hemispheric electroencephalographic and attentional correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 591-594.

Kiernan, B. D., Dane, J. R., Phillips, L. H., & Price, D. D. (1995). Hypnoanalgesia reduces R-III nocioceptive reflex: Further evidence concerning the multifactorial nature of hypnotic analgesia. Pain, 60, 39-47.

Kropotov, J. D., Crawford, H. J., & Polyakov, Y. I. (1997). Somatosensory event-related potential changes to painful stimuli during hypnotic analgesia: Anterior cingulate cortex and anterior temporal cortex intracranial recordings. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 1-8.

Lamas, J. R., & Crawford, H. J. (1997). Auditory event-related potentials and attention: Effects of posthypnotic suggested deafness and hypnotizability level. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 25, 72-73.

Lamas, J. R., & Crawford, H. J. (1998). Counting deviant tones in odd-ball paradigm: Auditory event-related potential amplitudes and latencies are differentially impacted in high and low hypnotizable persons. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 98.

Lamas, J. R., Crawford, H. J., & Vendemia, J. M. C. (1998). Processing irrelevant auditory stimuli during oddball paradigm: Hypnotizability level moderates event-related potential amplitudes and latencies. Manuscript under review.

Meier, W., Klucken, M., Soyka, D., & Bromm, B. (1993). Hypnotic hypo- and hyperanalgesia: divergent effects on pain ratings and pain-related cerebral potentials. Pain, 53, 175-181.

Melzack, R. (1992). Recent concepts of pain. Journal of Medicine, 13, 147-160.

Melzack, R., & Casey, K. L. (1968). Sensory, motivational, and central control determinants of pain. A new conceptual model. In D. R. Kenshalo (Ed.), The Skin Senses (pp. 423-439). Springfield, IL: C.C. Thomas.

Melzack, R., & Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science, 150, 971-979.

Mészáros, I., Bányai, É. I., & Greguss, A. C. (1978). Alteration of activity level: The essence of hypnosis or a byproduct of the type of induction? In G. Adám, I. Mészáros, & É. I. Bányai (Eds.), Advanced Physiological Science, Brain and Behaviour, 17, 457-465.

Miltner, W., Johnson, Jr., R., Braun, C., & Larbig, W. (1989). Somatosensory event-related potentials to painful and non-painful stimuli: effects of attention. Pain, 38, 303-312.

Piccione, C., Hilgard, E. R., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1989). On the degree of stability of measured hypnotizability over a 25-year period. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 289-295.

Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attentional system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.

Price, D. D. (1988). Psychological and neural mechanisms of pain. New York: Raven.

Price, D. D. (1996). Hypnotic analgesia: Psychological and neural mechanisms. In J. Barber (Ed.), Hypnosis and suggestion in the treatment of pain: A clinical guide (pp. 67-84). New York: W. W. Norton & Co.

Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C. (1997). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science, 277, 968-971.

Rockstroh, B., Müller, M., Wagner, M., Cohen, R., & Elbert, T. (1993). "Probing" the nature of the CNV. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 87, 235-241.

Sabourin, M. E., Cutcomb, S. D., Crawford, H. J., & Pribram, K. H. (1990). EEG correlates of hypnotic susceptibility and hypnotic trance: Spectral analysis and coherence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 10, 125-142.

Santarcangelo, E. L., Busse, K., & Carli, G. (1989). Changes in electromyographically recorded human monosynaptic reflex in relation to hypnotic susceptibility and hypnosis. Neuroscience Letters, 104, 157-160.

Sharav, V., & Tal, M. (1989). Masseter inhibitory periods and sensations evoked by electrical tooth-pulp stimulation in subjects under hypnotic anesthesia. Brain Research, 479, 247-254.

Sherrington, C. (1947). The integrative action of the nervous system. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Skinner, J. E., & Yingling, C. D. (1977). Central gating mechanisms that regulate event-related potentials and behavior: A neural model for attention. In J. E. Desmedt (ed.), Attention, voluntary contraction and event-related cerebral potentials. Progress in clinical neurophysiology (Vol. 1, pp. 28-68). Basel: Karger.

Spiegel, D. (1991). Neurophysiological correlates of hypnosis and dissociation. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 3, 440-445.

Spiegel, D., & Albert, L. H. (1983). Naloxone fails to reverse hypnotic alleviation of chronic pain. Psychopharmacology, 81, 140-143.

Spiegel, D., Bierre, P., & Rootenberg, J. (1989). Hypnotic alteration of somatosensory perception. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 749-754.

Sternbach, R. A. (1982). On strategies for identifying neurochemical correlates of hypnotic analgesia: A brief communication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 30, 251-256.

Stevenson, J. B. (1978). Reversal of hypnosis-induced analgesia by naloxone. Lancet, 2, 991-992.

Talbot, J. D., Marrett, S., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E., Bushnell, M. C., & Duncan, G. H. (1991). Multiple representations of pain in human cerebral cortex. Science, 251, 1355-1358.

Tecce, J. J., & Cattanach, L. (1982). Contingent negative variation. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications and related fields (pp. 543-562). Baltimore: Urban and Schwarzenberg.

Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268-277.

Wik, G., Fischer, H., Bragee, B., Finer, B., & Fredrikson, M. (1998). Functional neuroanatomy of hypnotic analgesia: A PET study of patients with fibromyalgia. Manuscript under review.

Willis, W. D., & Westlund, K. N. (1997). Neuroanatomy of the pain system and of the pathways that modulate pain. Journal of clinical Neurophysiology, 14, 2-31.

Yamaguchi, S., & Knight, R. T. (1990). Gating of somatosensory input by human prefrontal cortex. Brain Research, 521, 281-288.

Zachariae, R., & Bjerring, P. (1990). The effect of hypnotically induced analgesia on flare reaction of the cutaneous histamine prick test. Archives of Dermatological Research, 282, 539-543.

Zachariae, R., & Bjerring, P. (1994). Laser-induced pain-related brain potentials and sensory pain ratings in high and low hypnotizable subjects during hypnotic suggestions of relaxation, dissociated imagery, focused analgesia, and placebo. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 56-80.

Zachariae, R., Bjerring, P., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Nielsen, T., & Gotliebsen, K. (1991). The effect of hypnotically induced emotional states on brain potentials evoked by painful argon laser stimulation. Clinical Journal of Pain, 7, 130-138.

Back to the top.


Back to the top.


| Discussion Board | Previous Page | Your Symposium |
Crawford, H.J.; Horton, J.E.; McClain-Furmanski, D.; Vendemia, J.; (1998). Brain Dynamic Shifts During The Elimination Of Perceived Pain And Distress: Neuroimaging Studies of Hypnotic Analgesia. Presented at INABIS '98 - 5th Internet World Congress on Biomedical Sciences at McMaster University, Canada, Dec 7-16th. Invited Symposium. Available at URL http://www.mcmaster.ca/inabis98/woody/crawford0611/index.html
© 1998 Author(s) Hold Copyright