***************
Invited Symposium: What Can Genetic Models Tell Us About Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)?






Abstract

Section 1

Section 2

Section 3

Section 4

Section 5




Discussion
Board

INABIS '98 Home Page Your Session Symposia & Poster Sessions Plenary Sessions Exhibitors' Foyer Personal Itinerary New Search

Pathologies of Brain Attentional Networks


Contact Person: Andrea Berger (andreab@oregon.uoregon.edu)


Introduction

Attention is currently being examine in terms of three major functions: orienting to sensory stimuli, executive functions, and maintaining the alert state (Posner & Raichle, 1996). Although knowledge of the precise neural mechanisms responsible for these operations is still incomplete, many of the brain areas and networks involved have been identified. Moreover, there is evidence that damage to some of these networks produces similar symptoms regardless of whether the etiology is stroke, degenerative disease, psychopathology or abnormality of development (Fernandez-Duque & Posner, in process; DiGirolamo & Posner, 1996). For this reason, a knowledge of the networks of attention may be of special use in examination of disorders with attentional symptoms whose cause is not well understood, such as ADHD. In this paper we will briefly examine the three networks in light of new findings that might relate to theories of ADHD.

Many attempts have been made to understand and characterize the deficits associate with ADHD. Recently, three different theoretical accounts have been proposed (Barkely, 1998; Sergeant et al., in press; Swanson et al., 1998a). We attempt to argue that it is possible to conceptualize all three under the umbrella of pathologies of attentional networks.

Back to the top.


Networks of attention

Posner and Raichle (1996) summarized the three attentional networks approach, and based the localization of the different networks on the brain-imaging literature. Each attentional function is localized not in a single brain area, but as a network of interconnected brain areas.

Executive-control network. This network has been related to the control of goal directed behavior, target detection, error detection, conflict resolution and inhibition of automatic responses. The executive control network seems to include the midline frontal areas including the anterior cingulate gyrus, SMA, and portions of the basal ganglia. Neuroimaging studies have shown activity in this area during tasks that require mental effort such as in dealing with conflict, handling novelty, developing anticipations and detecting errors (Posner & DiGirolamo, 1998). Recently it has been shown that tasks involving both cognitive and emotional controls produce activation in the cingulate (Bush et al., 1998; Posner & Rothbart, in press).

Accumulating evidence shows involvement of the basal ganglia, more specifically the caudate nucleus, in cognitive functioning (Beiser, et al., 1997). Many studies showed similar behavioral deficits in animals following experimental lesions of the anterior dorsolateral frontal cortex and the caudate. Moreover, the caudate and the prefrontal cortex showed similar activity when recorded with depth electrodes during tasks as lexical decision and semantic categorization, that were not due to motor or premotor programming (Abdullaev et al., in press). In addition to that, the basal ganglia have been considered to be particularly important in mediating the connection between executive attention and other attentional networks (Jackson, Marrocco & Posner, 1994; LaBerge, 1990).

Lateral areas of the frontal cortex have also often been identified with executive attention. However, the bulk of the literature suggests to us that these areas involve representation of specific kinds of spatial, verbal or form information rather than more general attentional operations.

Alerting network. The alerting network is defined by a network of brain areas in the right frontal lobe (especially the superior region of Brodmann area 6), the right parietal lobe and the locus ceruleus (Posner & Petersen, 1990; Posner & Raichle, 1996). This network is involved in establishing a vigilant state and maintaining readiness to react. Recent alert monkey studies have shown clearly that the readiness induced by warning signals can be blocked by drugs that reduce norepineperine (Marrocco & Davidson, 1998). In addition patients with right parietal lesions show difficulty in sustaining attention and in the use of warning signals to improve behavior (Robertson et al., 1995).

Orienting network. A network for covert orienting to sensory, particularly visual signals, has been discussed in some detail previously (Posner & Dehaene, 1994). (Neuroimaging evidence has shown that covert shifts of visual attention most strongly activates the parietal lobe Moreover, other areas, related also to the oculo-motor system, are also activated (Corbetta, 1998). There is strong evidence that attending to a spatial location increases blood flow and electrical activity in extrastriate visual regions particularly the fusiform gyrus (Magnun, et al., in press).

Back to the top.


Leading theories of ADHD

As mentioned in the introduction, three theoretical accounts explaining ADHD have been recently proposed. Swanson et al. (1998a) charaterized ADHD as a combination of executive attention and alerting deficits. This approach focuses on two of the attentional networks and predicts brain pathology in areas related to these networks, i.e., midline frontal cortex (cingulate and SMA), basal ganglia (especially caudate), anterior prefrontal cortex, anterior right parietal cortex. According to Barkley (1998), the core of ADHD pathology are Executive Functions. By executive functions he referred to working memory, internalization of self-directed speech, control of emotion arousal and motivation. Barkley suggests that during normal development children switch from external cues to internal guidance of behavior. Although the definition of executive functions varies somewhat in the literature, most of the functions included by Barkley have been conceptualized, as being part of the executive attention network. Activation of both emotional and cognitive tasks has been shown to occur in the anterior cingulate (Bush et al., 1998). Posner & Rothbart (in press) have argued that emotional control mechanisms arise in early infancy and this same general system is later used for control of conflict and other executive functions. Barkely, too, although using a different nomenclature, ties the dishinhibition behavioral symptoms of ADHD as being secondary to the main executive control deficit. Sergeant et al., (in press), on the other hand, emphasize the energetic factors, as the most critical deficit in ADHD. According to their framework, ADHD suffer from a deficit in the energetic maintenance and allocation of resources (which leads to the secondary symptoms of dishinhibitory behavior). This approach identifies the activation pool with the basal ganglia and corpus striatum, and suggest it operates mainly through criteria shifts in the criterion for responding. According to an attentional network framework, this frontal activation could, too, be part of the executive control network.

Back to the top.


Brain pathologies in ADHD

Filipek (Filipek, 1996; Filipek et al., 1997) found that despite similar hemispheric volumes, ADHD subjects had smaller volumes of left caudate and caudate head, with reverse asymmetry than controls and right anterior-superior frontal region en bloc and white matter. Moreover, possible structural correlates of ADHD response to stimulants were found. Castellanos et al. (1996) found smaller right anterior frontal, caudate and globus pallidus regions in ADHD compared to control. Aylward et al. (1996), too, report volumetric differences in caudate and globus pallidus, between ADHD children and control (for review, see Swanson et al. (1998b)).

Behavioral studies support the basic idea that ADHD is a deficit in executive control and regulation that extends to emotional and cognitive processes (Barkley, 1998). There is evidence of difficulty in controlling the activation state (Sergeant et al., 1998). This result may relate to basal ganglia and frontal control system deficits, rather than more direct NE or parietal abnormalities. There is little empirical support of the involvement of the orienting network in ADHD pathology. Although a few studies have tested endogenous and exogenous orienting of attention, in ADHD, the results are ambiguous as to abnormalities in the basal ganglia and frontal control structures, versus parietal structures.

Back to the top.


Conclusions

For many years, psychiatrists and psychologists have been trying to define and understand the deficits that underlie ADHD. Evidence is consistent with difficulties in two of the attentional networks: executive functions / effortful control, and vigilance and alerting regulation. We believe that a better understanding of the functions and neuroanatomical bases of attentional networks in normal development, will be helpful in understanding ADHD.

References

Abdullaev , Y.G., Bechtereva, N.P. and Melnichuk, K.V. (in press). Neuronal activity of the human caudate nucleus and prefrontal cortex in cognitive tasks. Behavioral Brain Research.

Aylward, E.H., Reiss, A.L., Reader, M.J., Singer, H.S., Brown, J.E., Denkla M.B. (1996). Basal Ganglia Volumes in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, 11:112-115.

Barkley, R.A. (1998). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Scientific American, 279:66-71 .

Beiser, D. G., Hua, S. E., & C., H. J. (1997). Network models of the basal gangila. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 185-190

Bush, G., Whalen, P.J., Rosen, B.R., Jenike, M.A., McInerey, S.C. and Rauch, S.L. (1998). The Counting Stroop: An Interference Task Specialized fro Funcional Neuroimaging - Validation Study With Functional MRI, Human Brain Mapping, 6(4): 270-282.

Castellanos, F.X., Giedd, J.N., March, W.L., Hamburger, S.D., Vaituzis, A.C., Dickstein, D.P., Sarfatti, S.E., Vauss, Y.C., Snell, J.W. and Lange, N. (1996). Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention- deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 53: 607-616.

Corbetta, M. (1998). Fronto-parietal Cortical Networks for Directing Attention and Eye to Visual Locations: Identical, Independent or Overlapping Neural Systems. Proc. of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95: 831-838.

DiGirolamo, G.J. and Posner, M.I. (1996). Attention and Schzophrenia: A View from Cognitive Neuroscience. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(2): 95-102.

Fernandez-Duque, D. and Posner, M.I. (in process) Imaging attentional networks in normal and brain injured populations.

Filipek, P.A. , Semrud-Clikeman, M., Steingard, R.J., Renshaw, P.F., Kennedy, D.N. and Biederman, J. (1997). Volumetric MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with normal controls. Neurology, 48: 589-601.

Filipek, P. A. (1996) Structural variations in measures in the developmental disorders. in Thatcher, Robert W. (Ed), Lyon, G. Reid (Ed), et al. Developmental neuroimaging: Mapping the development of brain and behavior. San Diego, CA, USA: Academic Press.

Jackson, S.R., Marrocco, R. and Posner, M.I. (1994). Networks of Anatomical Areas Controlling Visuospatial Attention. Neural Networks, 7:925-944.

LaBerge, D. (1990) Thalamic and cortical mechanisms of attention suggested by recent positron emission tomographic experiments. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2: 358-372.

Mangun, G.R. (in press). Integrating Imaging and Electrophysiology. in Michael Gazzaniga (ed) The Cognitive Neurosciences (Revised)

Marrocco, R.T. and Davidson, M.C. (1998). Neurochemistry of Attention. in Raja Parasuraman (ed) The Attentive Brain. The MIT press, Cambridge, England.

Posner, M.I. and Dahaene, S. (1994). Attentional Networks. Trends in Neurosciences, 17(2): 75-79.

Posner, M.I. and DiGirolamo, G.J. (1998). Executive Attention: Conflict, Target Detection, and Cognitive Control. in Raja Parasuraman (ed) The Attentive Brain. The MIT press, Cambridge, England.

Posner, M.I. and Petersen, S.E. (1990). the Attention System of the Human Brain. Ann. Rev. Neuro. 13: 25-42.

Posner, M.I. and Raichle, M. (1996) Images of Mind (Revised). Scientific American Books, Washington, DC.

Posner, M.I. and Rothbart, M.K. (in press). Attention, self regulation and consciousness. Phil. Trans. Royal Society Lon., 353:1-13.

Robertson, I. H, Tegner, R., Tham, K., Lo, A.; et al.(1995) Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: Theoretical and rehabilitation implications. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology. 17(3):416-430.

Sergeant, J., Oosterlann, J. and van der Meere, J. (in press). Information processing and energetic factors in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. in Herbert C. Quay and Ann E. Hogan (eds) Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Disorders. New York: Plenum.

Swanson, J., Posner, M.I., Cantwell, D., Wigal, S., Crinella, F., Filipek, P.A., Emerson, J. Tucker, D., Nalcioglu, O. (1998a). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: symptom domain, cognitive processes and neural networks. In Raja Parasuraman (ed) The Attentive Brain. The MIT press, Cambridge, England.

Swanson, J., Castellanos, F.X., Murias, M., LaHoste, G. and Kennedy, J. (1998b). Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(2): 263-271.

Back to the top.


| Discussion Board | Previous Page | Your Symposium |
Berger, A.; Posner, M.I.; (1998). Pathologies of Brain Attentional Networks. Presented at INABIS '98 - 5th Internet World Congress on Biomedical Sciences at McMaster University, Canada, Dec 7-16th. Invited Symposium. Available at URL http://www.mcmaster.ca/inabis98/sadile/berger0855/index.html
© 1998 Author(s) Hold Copyright