Neural Bases of Hypnosis


Re^3: Symposium 611-Thanx

Helen Crawford



Dear Harry,

Make it surprising, not scarey.  Consider the number of cultures that actively use behavioral approaches to eliminate the perception of pain and distress.  The fakirs of India are but one example.  Larbig in Germany has published several articles on EEG/ERPs recorded during hanging from steel hooks places in the back .... he observed substantial increases in theta during their analgesic state!  This is similar to the increased theta we have observed.

May the medical field begin using behavioral approaches such as hypnotic analgesia more systematically!  An excellent introduction is the book by the Hilgards.  Jack (E.R.) Hilgard is a professor emeritus at Stanford University, past president of the American Psychological Association, and a member of the National Academy of Sciences.  His spouse and colleague Josie Hilgard was a psychiatrist/psychologist.  The latest edition of their book is:  

Ernest R. Hilgard and Josephine R. Hilgard (1994).
Hypnosis in the relief of pain (rev. ed.).  New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc.

Another great one is:
 
Hilgard, E. R. (1986).  
Divided consciousness:  Multiple controls in human thought and action (rev. ed.).  New York: Wiley.
 
____________________________________________---
____________________________________________


On Mon Dec 14, grover wrote
---------------------------
>Dr. Crawford:  Thanx  for the detailed response.  The last part was scary - at least to me.  Cheers -akg.
>On Thu Dec 10, Helen Crawford wrote
>-----------------------------------
>>On Fri Dec 4, grover wrote
>>--------------------------
>>>Dr.Crawford: Great presentation.  Hope you have fun at the meeting.  In hypnotic analgesia, is it  that the pain sensation is not received or is it simply not processed?  What are the side effects? Are  any other activities from the area also affected and is pain from other areas received nevertheless?  Is there a selectivity for the tissue/organ?
>>>

Crawford's response:

>>The sensory information from the noxious stimulus is of course registered in the brain and processed (erps are still present), but the intriguing result is that this sensory information (which is usually perceived as painful and/or distressful) does not reach conscious awareness.  This general work is very important for our understanding the emergence (or non-emergence) of consciousness (which has multiple meanings of course).  

>>We argue that the experimental data provide evidence that hypnotic analgesia is an active inhibitory process that operates to reallocate thalamocortical activities as evidenced in our rCBF (Crawford et al., 1993), ERP (Crawford et al., 1998; other papers; AND numerous ones in the lit.), fMRI (Crawford et al., 1998), ERP (Crawford et al., 1998; other papers; AND numerous ones in the lit); and EEG (from Chen, Karlin, and my labs).  I am most pleased to see Rainville using PET to replicate and extend our research with our prior rCBF and fMRI studies.  There is a paper in press in the European J. of Pain from Mats Fredrikson's PET lab that show similar changes in fibromyalgia patients. These inhibitory processes are far reaching as exemplified by work that demonstrates hypnotic analgesia may extend as far as spinal cord antinociceptive mechanisms as evidenced by reductions in brief latency (Hagbarth & Finer, 1963) and R-III amplitude (Kiernan, Dane, Phillips, & Price, 1995) of spinal reflexes (for an exception, see Santarcangelo, Busse, & Carli, 1989).  A recent study in PAIN from cooperative research between Carli's lab in Sienna, Italy, and a French group shows either increased or decreased spinal reflex changes in subjects experiencing complete hypnotic analgesia; I am not sure I referenced it, so here it is:

>>Danziger, N., Fournier, E., Bouhassira, D., Michaud, D., De Broucker, T., Santacangelo, E., Carli, G., Chertock, L., & Willer, J. C. (1998).  Different strategies of modulation can be operative during hypnotic analgesia: a neurophysiological study.  Pain, 75, 85-92.

>>We have not seen negative side effects from our experimental research. But we see positive side effects.  The clinical literature on the use of hypnotic analgesia with chronic and acute pain patients demonstrates over and over that the successful use of hypnotic analgesia leads to increased self-efficacy and wellbeing of the whole individual.  For instance, with a group of adults with chronic low back pain, we found the teaching of experimental pain control (cold pressor and electrical stimulation) was highly effective.  The majority were surprisingly moderately to highly hypnotizable.  As we report in the Int. J. of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis special issue on chronic pain (Jan. 1998; also see Oct. 1997 issue as this is a two-part special issue):

>>"Participating in an experimental study involving the learning of control over experimental pain resulted in transfer of the control to experienced low back pain as well as improved psychological well-being in daily living.  Pain experienced at the time of arrival at the experiment decreased significantly.  Individuals with more pain tend to report poorer sleep quality and more awakenings during the night (e.g., Affleck, Urrows, Tennen, Higgins, & Abeles, 1996).  Overall, our participants reported significant enhancements in sleep quality, as reflected by reduced time to fall asleep, over the experimental period.  Similarly, self-reported depression reduced significantly and psychological health increased significantly.  Finally, use of medications reduced significantly.  

>>"The experiment demonstrates the importance of developing self-efficacy through the learning of experimental pain control and the understanding of one's own attentional and disattentional abilities.  Our data provide experimental support to Brown and Fromm's (1987) introduction of experimental pain control as a first step in enhancing self-efficacy in chronic pain management.  Furthermore, it argues for the early introduction of behavioral techniques such as hypnosis and relaxation before the development of chronic pain (Crawford, 1995a,b).  Already our experimental pain training approach using the cold pressor test is being used in clinical settings in the United States (e.g., Holroyd, 1996) and Europe (P. Alden, personal communication, October 1996)."

>>Dependent upon the type of clinical pain and whether it is important to the patient to know if the pain is present or not, some clinicians (e.g., Craselneck) will provide suggestions of reduction but not elimination of pain.  In other words, a little bit of pain is sometimes important to have.  
>>
>>You ask: "Are  any other activities from the area also affected and is pain from other areas received nevertheless?"  This is a very intriguing and provocative two-part question.  And needs further investigation!  The elimination of pain can be localized or more general.  Pain from other areas may be perceived even though pain to the localized area is not.  Furthermore, we have observed individuals with no sensation of pain or distress actively move and observe the area which is hypnotically anesthesized and maintain the anesthesia.  Some years ago Martin Orne and I consulted on a documentary on hypnosis -- there is a anesthesiologist from UCLA Medical School who placed a sterilized needle through part of his hand, then moves and comments on it, but feels no sensations of pain or distress.    


>>  
>


[ Previous ] [ Next ] [ Index ]           Wed Dec 16